Livewired. David Eagleman

Читать онлайн.
Название Livewired
Автор произведения David Eagleman
Жанр Биология
Серия
Издательство Биология
Год выпуска 0
isbn 9781838850975



Скачать книгу

stump becomes finer. Touch can now be sensed with lighter pressure, and two touches close together can be sensed as separate touches rather than a single one. Because the brain is now devoting more territory to the remaining, undamaged areas, sensing becomes higher resolution.

image

      Neural redeployment replaces the old paradigm of predetermined brain areas with something more flexible. Territory can be reassigned to different tasks. There is nothing special about visual cortex neurons, for example. They are simply neurons that happen to be involved in processing edges or colors in people who have functioning eyes. These exact same neurons can process other types of information in the sightless.

      The old paradigm would assert that the North American acreage labeled as Louisiana was predetermined for French people. The new paradigm is not surprised when the Louisiana Territory gets sold and citizens from around the globe set up shop there.

      Given that the brain has to distribute all its tasks across the finite volume of the cortex, it may be that some disorders arise from suboptimal distributions. One example is autistic savantism, in which a child who has severe cognitive and social deficits might be a virtuoso at, say, memorizing the phone book, or copying down visual scenes, or solving the Rubik’s Cube with stunning speed. The pairing of cognitive disabilities with outstanding talents has attracted many theories; one of relevance here is an unusual distribution of cortical real estate.36 The idea is that atypical feats can be accomplished when the brain devotes an unusually large swath of its real estate to one task (such as memorization, or visual analysis, or puzzles). But these human superpowers come at the expense of other tasks among which brains normally divide their territory, such as all the subtasks that add up to reliable social skills.

      Recent decades have yielded several revelations about brain plasticity, but perhaps the biggest surprise is its rapidity. Some years ago, researchers at McGill University put several adults who had just recently lost their sight into a brain scanner. The participants were asked to listen to sounds. Not surprisingly, the sounds caused activity in their auditory cortex. But the sounds also caused activity in their occipital cortex—activity that would not have been there even a few weeks earlier, when the participants had sight. The activity wasn’t as strong as that seen in people who had been blind for a long while, but it was detectable nevertheless.37

      This demonstrated that the brain can implement changes rapidly when vision disappears. But how rapidly?

      The researcher Alvaro Pascual-Leone began to wonder about the speed at which these major brain changes can take place. He noted that aspiring instructors at a school for the blind were required to blindfold themselves for seven full days to gain firsthand understanding of their students’ living experiences. Most of the instructors become aware of enhanced skills with sounds—orienting to them, judging their distance, and identifying them:

      Several describe becoming able to identify people quickly and accurately as they started talking or even as they simply walked by due to the cadence of their steps. Several learned to differentiate cars by the sounds of their motors, and one described the “joy of telling motorcycles apart by their sound.”38

      This got Pascual-Leone and his colleagues considering what would happen if a sighted person were blindfolded in a laboratory setting for several days. They launched the experiment, and what they found was nothing short of remarkable. They discovered that neural reorganization—the same kind seen in blind subjects—also happens with temporary blindness of sighted subjects. Rapidly.

      In one of their studies, sighted participants were blindfolded for five days, during which time they were put through an intensive Braille-training paradigm.39 At the end of five days, the subjects had become quite good at detecting subtle differences between Braille characters—much better than a control group of sighted participants who underwent the same training without a blindfold.

      But especially striking was what happened to their brains, as measured in the scanner. Within five days, the blindfolded participants had recruited their occipital cortex when they were touching objects. Control subjects, not surprisingly, used only their somatosensory cortex. The blindfolded subjects also showed occipital responses to sounds and words.

      When this new occipital lobe activity was intentionally disrupted in the laboratory by magnetic pulses, the Braille-reading advantage of the blindfolded subjects went away—indicating that the recruitment of this brain area was not an accidental side effect but a critical piece of the improved behavioral performance.

      When the blindfold was removed, the response of the occipital cortex to touch or sound disappeared within a day. At that point, the participants’ brains returned to looking indistinguishable from every other sighted brain out there.

      In another study, the visual areas of the brain were carefully mapped out using more powerful neuroimaging techniques. Participants were blindfolded, put in a scanner, and asked to perform a touching task that required fine discrimination with their fingers. In these conditions, investigators could detect activity emerging in the primary visual cortex after a blindfolding session of a mere forty to sixty minutes.40

      The shock of these findings was their sheer speed. The shape shifting of brains is not like the glacial drifting of continental plates, but can instead be remarkably swift. In later chapters, we’ll see that visual deprivation causes the unmasking of already-existing nonvisual input into the occipital cortex, and we’ll come to understand how the brain is always sprung like a mousetrap to implement rapid change. But for now the important point is that the brain’s changes are more brisk than even the most optimistic neuroscientist would have dared to guess at the beginning of this century.

image

      Let’s zoom back out to the bigger picture. Just as sharp teeth and fast legs are useful for survival, so is neural flexibility: it allows brains to optimize performance in a variety of environments.

      But the competition in the brain has a potential downside as well. Whenever there’s an imbalance of activity in the senses, a potential takeover can happen, and it can happen rapidly. A redistribution of resources can be optimal when a limb or a sense has been permanently amputated or lost, but the rapid conquest of territory may have to be actively combated in other scenarios. And this consideration led me and my former student Don Vaughn to propose a new theory for what happens to brains in the dark of night.

      One of the unsolved mysteries in neuroscience is why brains dream. What are these bizarre nighttime hallucinations about? Do they have meaning? Or are they simply random neural activity in search of a coherent narrative? And why are dreams so richly visual, igniting the occipital cortex every night into a conflagration of activity?

      Consider the following: In the chronic and unforgiving competition for brain real estate, the visual system has a unique problem to deal with. Because of the rotation of the planet, it is cast into darkness for an average of twelve hours every cycle. (This refers to 99.9999 percent of our species’ evolutionary history, not to the current, electricity-blessed times.) We’ve already seen that sensory deprivation triggers neighboring territories to take over. So how does the visual system deal with this unfair disadvantage?

      By keeping the occipital cortex active during the night.

      We suggest that dreaming exists to keep the visual cortex from being taken over by neighboring areas. After all, the rotation of the planet does not affect anything about your ability to touch, hear, taste, or smell; only vision suffers in the dark. As a result, the visual cortex finds itself in danger every night of a takeover by the other senses. And given the startling rapidity with which changes in territory can happen (remember the forty to sixty minutes we just saw), the threat is formidable. Dreams are the means by which the visual cortex prevents takeover.

      To better understand this, let’s zoom out. Although a sleeper looks as though he is relaxed and shut