Lifespan Development. Tara L. Kuther

Читать онлайн.
Название Lifespan Development
Автор произведения Tara L. Kuther
Жанр Зарубежная психология
Серия
Издательство Зарубежная психология
Год выпуска 0
isbn 9781544332253



Скачать книгу

enjoy playing games that involve feats of coordination, such as running while kicking a ball. By age 5, most North American children can throw and catch a ball, climb a ladder, and ride a tricycle. Some 5-year-olds can even skate or ride a bicycle (Gabbard, 2012).

      Young children’s motor abilities are also influenced by their context. For example, young children of some nations can swim in rough ocean waves that many adults of other nations would not attempt. Advances in gross motor skills help children move about and develop a sense of mastery of their environment, but it is fine motor skills that permit young children to take responsibility for their own care.

      Fine Motor Skills

      As children grow older, their fine motor skills improve. The ability to button a shirt, pour milk into a glass, assemble puzzles, and draw pictures all involve eye–hand and small muscle coordination. As children get better at these skills, they are able to become more independent and do more for themselves. Young children become better at grasping eating utensils and become more self-sufficient at feeding. Many fine motor skills are very difficult for young children because they involve both hands and both sides of the brain. Tying a shoelace is a complex act requiring attention, memory for an intricate series of hand movements, and the dexterity to perform them. Although preschoolers struggle with this task, by 5 to 6 years of age, most children can tie their shoes (Payne, Isaacs, & Larry, 2016). Recent research suggests that children’s fine motor ability influences cognition—specifically, their ability to use their fingers to aid in counting predicts their mathematical skills (Fischer, Suggate, Schmirl, & Stoeger, 2018).

A young child climbs the jungle gym at a park. His mother stands a few feet away and watches.

      Climbing requires strength, coordination, and balance.

      Michele Oenbrink / Alamy Stock Photo

      Young children’s emerging fine motor skills enable them to draw using large crayons and, eventually, pencils. Drawing reflects the interaction of developmental domains: physical (fine motor control) and cognitive (planning skills, spatial understanding, and the recognition that pictures can symbolize objects, people, and events) (Yamagata, 2007). Although toddlers simply scribble when given a crayon, by 3 years of age, children‘s scribbles become more controlled, often recognizable, pictures. A human figure is often drawn as a tadpole-like figure with a circle for the head with eyes and sometimes a smiley mouth and then a line or two beneath to represent the rest of the body. Tadpole-like forms are characteristic of young children’s art in all cultures (Cox, 1993). Most 3-year-olds can draw circles, squares, rectangles, triangles, crosses, and Xs and they begin to combine shapes into more complex designs. Between 3 and 4, young children begin to understand the representational function of drawings, and even when drawings appear to be nothing more than scribbles, young children often label them as representing a particular object and remember the label. In one study, children were asked to draw a balloon and a lollipop. The drawings looked the same to adults, but the children were adamant about which was which (Bloom, 2000), suggesting that it is important to ask a child what his or her drawing is rather than guess, because children’s creations reflect their perspectives. Between ages 4 and 5, children’s drawings loosely begin to depict actual objects, demonstrating the convergence of fine motor skills and the cognitive development of representational ability. As shown in Figure 7.1, human figures typically include a torso, arms, legs, faces, and soon hands. As cognitive and fine motor skills improve, children create more sophisticated drawings of the human form. Table 7.1 summarizes milestones of gross and fine motor skill development in young children.

A stick figure of a person that includes a head, arm, hands, legs, and feet.

      Figure 7.1 A Typical 2- to 3-Year-Old’s Drawing of a Person

      Source: Claire Marley, 2009.

      Thinking in Context 7.1

      1 Children who suffer brain injuries often regain some, and sometimes all, of their capacities. How might you explain this, given what you have learned about brain development?

      2 How might contextual factors such as neighborhood, family, school, and culture influence the development of motor skills?

      Cognitive-Developmental and Sociocultural Reasoning in Early Childhood

      Four-year-old Timothy stands up on his toes and releases his parachute toy, letting the action figure dangle from a parachute drift a few feet from him and collapse on the floor. “I’m going to go up high and make it faster,” he says, imagining standing on the sofa and making the toy sail far into the clouds. He stands on the sofa and releases the toy, which sails a bit further this time. “Next time he’ll jump out of the plane even higher!” Timothy thinks, excitedly. His friend Isaiah calls out, “Let’s make him land on the moon! He can meet space people!”

      Timothy and Isaiah can plan, think of solutions to problems, and use language to communicate their ideas. They learn through play by interacting with people and objects around them. From the cognitive-developmental perspective, young children’s thought progresses from the sensory and motor schemes of infancy to more sophisticated representational thought.

      Piaget’s Cognitive-Developmental Theory: Preoperational Reasoning

      According to Piaget, preoperational reasoning appears in young children from about ages 2 to 6. Preoperational reasoning is characterized by a dramatic leap in the use of symbolic thinking that permits young children to use language, interact with others, and play using their own thoughts and imaginations to guide their behavior. It is symbolic thought that enables Timothy and Isaiah to use language to communicate their thoughts and desires—and it is also what allows them to send their toy on a mission to the moon to visit with pretend space people.

      Characteristics of Preoperational Reasoning

      Young children in the preoperational stage show impressive advances in representational thinking, but they are unable to grasp logic and cannot understand complex relationships. Children who show preoperational reasoning tend to make several common errors, including egocentrism, animism, centration, and irreversibility.

      Egocentrism

      “See my picture?” Ricardo asks as he holds up a blank sheet of paper. Mr. Seris answers, “You can see your picture, but I can’t. Turn your page around so that I can see your picture. There it is! It’s beautiful,” he proclaims after Ricardo flips the piece of paper, permitting him to see his drawing. Ricardo did not realize that even though he could see his drawing, Mr. Seris could not. Ricardo displays egocentrism, the inability to take another person’s point of view or perspective. The egocentric child views the world from his or her own perspective, assuming that other people share her feelings, knowledge, and even physical view of the world.

      A classic task used to illustrate preoperational children’s egocentrism is the three-mountain task. As shown in Figure 7.2, the child sits at a table facing three large mountains. A teddy bear is placed in a chair across the table from the child. The child is asked how the mountains look to the teddy bear. Piaget found that young children in the preoperational stage demonstrated egocentrism because they described the scene from their own perspective rather than the teddy bear’s. They did not understand that the teddy bear would have a different view of the mountains (Piaget & Inhelder, 1967).

A mountain display is on a large table. A young child and an adult stand at one end of the table. They can see a house on the mountainside. At the 
            </div>
      	</div>
  	</div>
  	<hr>
  	<div class=