Introducing Cognitive Analytic Therapy. Anthony Ryle

Читать онлайн.
Название Introducing Cognitive Analytic Therapy
Автор произведения Anthony Ryle
Жанр Психотерапия и консультирование
Серия
Издательство Психотерапия и консультирование
Год выпуска 0
isbn 9781119695134



Скачать книгу

Group, 2014). These figures also indicate the need to understand what sort of factors contribute to the greater remainder of the variance.

      But the implications of possible inherited characteristics for psychotherapy are considerable since they imply that a certain amount of what may be described as personality may be the effects of temperament rather than of developmental experience. As such they may be relatively immutable, raising the question of whether, in that case, the task of psychotherapy may be, in part, to help an individual to live with and manage their particular temperamental characteristics as well as to make sense of their consequences. This would apply also to those with established and disabling disorders (such as psychosis or anorexia) whatever their, possibly complex, origins The effects of temperament are rarely direct and will, importantly, include the complex effects whereby the behavior of a child will actually modify the responses of others and so their experience (Plomin, 1994, 2018), which will then, in turn, be internalized. These effects would also include the consequences of differing experiences within a group of siblings. Thus, a demandingly aggressive or a highly anxious child will elicit very different responses from a parent or siblings and peers compared to a more placid sibling. This mechanism (“non‐shared family environment”) accounts in part for the very different developmental experience which siblings may have within the same family. It should be noted also that certain temperamental characteristics may confer a degree of developmental resilience in whatever conditions a child develops, while others may do very well indeed but only in certain favorable conditions, the so‐called “dandelion–orchid” hypothesis (see review by Kennedy, 2013).

      These various features of our evolutionary inheritance, in particular our capacity to be shaped by developmental experience and the internalization of social meanings and cultural values, have largely contributed to the historic conceptual conflict between the protagonists of the effects of “nature” and “nurture” or of genes versus environment. This “for or against” argument should, by now, be essentially redundant. As Plomin (1994) has remarked, the “nature–nurture” debate is centered nowadays around the hyphen and around its developmental, synthetic interactions. And as we (AR) have previously noted, notwithstanding our various predispositions, “humans are above all biologically predisposed to be social formed.”

      In the view proposed here, although humans retain their biological characteristics, the sources of their evolutionary success are to be found in the ways in which they are radically unlike other animals. These include notably: (a) the enormously enlarged brains which enabled our ancestors to replace stereotypic and predetermined techniques with flexible, intelligent solutions in the struggle to wrest a living from nature; and (b) the development of faculties, eventually speech, which enhanced their ability to work together and to pass on knowledge from one generation to the next. As a result of these changes, cultural evolution became a dominant factor in how humankind evolved biologically. As new social forms radically altered the behaviors and qualities of individuals likely to aid survival of the group, individuals evolved who could learn the skills and values of the particular group they